Cohen's g is an effect size of asymmetry (or marginal heterogeneity) for
dependent (paired) contingency tables ranging between 0 (perfect symmetry)
and 0.5 (perfect asymmetry) (see stats::mcnemar.test()
). (Note this is not
not a measure of (dis)agreement between the pairs, but of (a)symmetry.)
Arguments
- x
a numeric vector or matrix.
x
andy
can also both be factors.- y
a numeric vector; ignored if
x
is a matrix. Ifx
is a factor,y
should be a factor of the same length.- ci
Confidence Interval (CI) level
- alternative
a character string specifying the alternative hypothesis; Controls the type of CI returned:
"two.sided"
(default, two-sided CI),"greater"
or"less"
(one-sided CI). Partial matching is allowed (e.g.,"g"
,"l"
,"two"
...). See One-Sided CIs in effectsize_CIs.- ...
Ignored
Value
A data frame with the effect size (Cohens_g
, Risk_ratio
(possibly with the prefix log_
), Cohens_h
) and its CIs (CI_low
and
CI_high
).
Confidence (Compatibility) Intervals (CIs)
Confidence intervals are based on the proportion (\(P = g + 0.5\))
confidence intervals returned by stats::prop.test()
(minus 0.5), which give
a good close approximation.
CIs and Significance Tests
"Confidence intervals on measures of effect size convey all the information
in a hypothesis test, and more." (Steiger, 2004). Confidence (compatibility)
intervals and p values are complementary summaries of parameter uncertainty
given the observed data. A dichotomous hypothesis test could be performed
with either a CI or a p value. The 100 (1 - \(\alpha\))% confidence
interval contains all of the parameter values for which p > \(\alpha\)
for the current data and model. For example, a 95% confidence interval
contains all of the values for which p > .05.
Note that a confidence interval including 0 does not indicate that the null
(no effect) is true. Rather, it suggests that the observed data together with
the model and its assumptions combined do not provided clear evidence against
a parameter value of 0 (same as with any other value in the interval), with
the level of this evidence defined by the chosen \(\alpha\) level (Rafi &
Greenland, 2020; Schweder & Hjort, 2016; Xie & Singh, 2013). To infer no
effect, additional judgments about what parameter values are "close enough"
to 0 to be negligible are needed ("equivalence testing"; Bauer & Kiesser,
1996).
Plotting with see
The see
package contains relevant plotting functions. See the plotting vignette in the see
package.
References
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd Ed.). New York: Routledge.
Examples
data("screening_test")
phi(screening_test$Diagnosis, screening_test$Test1)
#> Phi (adj.) | 95% CI
#> -------------------------
#> 0.85 | [0.81, 1.00]
#>
#> - One-sided CIs: upper bound fixed at [1.00].
phi(screening_test$Diagnosis, screening_test$Test2)
#> Phi (adj.) | 95% CI
#> -------------------------
#> 0.85 | [0.81, 1.00]
#>
#> - One-sided CIs: upper bound fixed at [1.00].
# Both tests seem comparable - but are the tests actually different?
(tests <- table(Test1 = screening_test$Test1, Test2 = screening_test$Test2))
#> Test2
#> Test1 "Neg" "Pos"
#> "Neg" 794 86
#> "Pos" 150 570
mcnemar.test(tests)
#>
#> McNemar's Chi-squared test with continuity correction
#>
#> data: tests
#> McNemar's chi-squared = 16.818, df = 1, p-value = 4.115e-05
#>
cohens_g(tests)
#> Cohen's g | 95% CI
#> ------------------------
#> 0.14 | [0.07, 0.19]
# Test 2 gives a negative result more than test 1!